According to the concept of if the majority of a persons interactions

Research examining the relationship between social support and psychological well-being has largely ignored the negative side of social interactions. However, empirical evidence suggests that negative interactions can potentially be more harmful than social support is helpful. This article critically reviews the literature investigating the relationship between social support and negative social interactions and their simultaneous effect on psychological well-being. A review of 28 studies revealed that there are conceptual, theoretical, and methodological limitations associated with this body of research. In order to unravel some of these limitations, studies are grouped according to three conceptual models: the additive effects model, the moderator model, and the domain-specific model. Finally, the article discusses directions social work practice research should take to tackle and fully appreciate the complexities of the relationship between social support and psychological well-being.

A long tradition of theory and research examines the relationship between social support and health. Research findings have repeatedly shown that people with spouses, family members, and friends who provide psychological and material support have better physical and mental health than those who have fewer resources. Mortality, morbidity, depression, and other undesirable health-related outcomes have been associated with the lack of social relationships (; ; ).

Another substantial body of literature has used a stress and coping theoretical framework to examine the role of social factors in health and well-being. The theory posits that adaptation is influenced by the stressful life situations to which a person is exposed, by the appraisal of such stressors, and by the psychological and social resources available to cope with them (; ; ; ). Given the importance of social resources, researchers are focusing on understanding the critical components of social networks and identifying their role in the stress and coping process.

Most research in the field of social support focuses on its positive aspects, such as instrumental, emotional, esteem, and tangible support. These findings are well established and quite promising. However, the focus on the positive aspects of social support obscures that there are also costs associated with social relations. The negative side of social relationships has received considerably less attention. A small, but growing body of research examining both positive and negative aspects of social support suggests that negative social interactions may be a distinct dimension of social support, which has a deleterious impact on mental health. Some investigations report that negative social interactions may, in fact, have more potent effects on psychological well-being than positive interactions (e.g., ; ; ). Other studies, however, report a stronger effect from positive interactions on psychological well-being (e.g., ), whereas still others report equal effects of the two constructs (e.g., ). These conflicting findings suggest that we must take a more balanced approach to investigating the relationship between social ties and mental health by including measures of both positive and negative social interactions. This is particularly important in research that informs social work practice and intervention.

This article provides a critical review of studies that examine the relationship between social support and psychological well-being. Specifically, studies investigating the dual nature of social support, both positive and negative interactions, and their simultaneous effect on psychological well-being are considered. The major focus of this review is to identify the conceptual and operational complexities of this body of research. This article also includes a limited discussion of the studies’ methodological complexities (e.g., sampling methods, populations). The goal of this review is fourfold: (1) to identify limitations in research examining the relationships among positive and negative social interactions and psychological well-being, (2) to identify ways of addressing these limitations, (3) to highlight three conceptual models for understanding the relationships among positive and negative interactions and well-being, and (4) to discuss implications for social work practice and research.

Positive and Negative Social Interactions and Psychological Well-Being

Although previous investigations have conceptualized negative interactions differently, most measures include in this category those actions by a member in one’s social network that cause distress (e.g., resentment, sadness, shame). Negative interactions may include discouraging the expression of feelings, making critical remarks, invading another’s privacy, interfering in another’s affairs, or failing to provide promised help, among others. This article excludes those costs discussed by social exchange theorists (e.g., ; ; , ), such as time or money, as well as actions that may be irritating but do not cause psychological harm. Instead, the focus is on those actions that are perceived as negative or problematic and that cause the individual to have some reservations about the relationship.

The same conceptual and methodological limitations that plague the research examining positive aspects of social relations have also affected research examining the negative aspects. These limitations may account for the equivocal nature of the findings in this area. One issue of concern is the variety of terms used to refer to what I call “negative social interactions.” The diversity in terms reflects the many ways in which negative social interactions are conceptualized. These terms include social conflict (; ; ), social hindrance (), social network stressors (), interpersonal stress (), problematic social interactions (; ; ; ), problematic relationships (), problematic support (), problematic social ties (), negative social ties (), negative social exchange (; ), negative social interactions (; ; ), negative social support (), negative network interactions (), negative relations (), network upset (; ; ), unwanted or unneeded interactions (), and social undermining (; ; ). All of these terms represent a broad range of negative social interactions that cause an individual to experience an adverse psychological reaction.

In addition to differences in terms, different sources of both positive and negative social interactions are found in the literature. Researchers have argued that greater attention should be paid to who provides support. Although positive and negative social interactions within family networks are emphasized in this article, the literature is more inclusive and considers other potential sources of interaction, including friends (), coworkers (), church members (), and roommates (). The inclusion of varied sources of interaction stems from the belief that different people provide different kinds of support with different degrees of effectiveness. A review of this research may help explain the inconsistencies in the literature.

One perspective assumes that positive and negative aspects of social relations are unrelated or have only a weak, insignificant association (see ). This perspective posits that positive and negative social interactions exert independent effects on psychological well-being, such that both types of interactions can occur simultaneously within relationships. But, studies examining the relationship between positive and negative social interactions yield inconsistent results. Discrepancies in the literature are not trivial, and clarifying the relationship between positive and negative social interactions is an important task. Some key questions include (1) whether positive and negative interactions are distinct from each other, and, if they are, the question remains whether they relate at all; (2) if positive and negative interactions are distinct and different, what is the relative potency of their effect on psychological well-being; and (3) whether positive and negative interactions demonstrate various interactions with psychological well-being. The nature of this relationship influences the form and effectiveness of social work interventions.

Review of the Literature

Articles for this review were identified through an electronic search of PsycINFO. All available dates were considered. Key words included social support, interpersonal interactions, and psychological well-being. Additional criteria restricted the sample to the adult population, and to investigations that included both positive and negative social interactions, and identified some dimension of psychological well-being as the outcome variable.

A review of the literature identified 28 studies; table A1 lists these studies alphabetically. This table functions as a summary of the literature in this area and highlights several important features of this research, including author(s) of the study; terms used to describe positive and negative social interactions; description of the sample, model, or theory that guided the study; statistical method used in the analysis; dependent variable(s); results; and correlation between positive and negative social interactions.

Table A1

Summary of Investigations of the Effects of Positive and Negative Social Interaction on Psychological Well-Being

Author(s)TermsSample NModelMethodDependent VariableResultsCorre-
lationSocial support, social conflict56 (college students; convenient)Additive effects, moderatorCorrelationsEmotional well-beingPositive and emotional well-being (+)YesNegative and emotional well-being (−)Positive × negative (significant)Negative, more potentSocial support, interpersonal stress349 (married adults; community)Additive effectsRegressionDepressionPositive and depression (−)…Negative and depression (+)Negative, more potentSupportive social inter- actions, problematic social interactions101 (medical students; convenient)Additive effectsRegressionLife satisfactionPositive and satisfaction (+)NoNegative and satisfaction (−)Negative, more potentSupportive interactions, problematic inter-actions71 (teen moms; convenient; 100% black)Additive effectsHierarchial multiple regressionPsychological symptomotologyNegative and distress (+)…Positive and distress (N.S.)Negative, more potentSupportive interactions, problematic inter-actions53 (teen moms; convenient; 100% black)Additive effects, moderatorHierarchical multiple regressionDepressionPositive and depression (−)…Negative and depression (N.S.)Positive × negative (significant)Negative, more potentPositive relations, negative relations429 families (inner-city; community; 66% black)Additive effects, moderatorStructural equation modelingDepressed affectPositive × stress (significant)…Negative × stress (significant)Positive social ties, negative social ties246 (elderly; convenient)Additive effectsStructural equation modelingPsychological well-being, psychological distressPositive and psychological well-being (N.S.)NoNegative and psychological well-being (−)Positive and distress (N.S.)Negative and distress (−)Negative, more potentNetwork helpfulness, network upset44 (caregivers; convenient)Additive effectsStepwise multiple regressionDepressionPositive and depression (N.S.)NoNegative and depression (+)Negative, more potentSocial support, social undermining285 (social workers; national probability; 100% black)Additive effectsMultiple regressionDepression, anxiety, somatic complaints, emotional exhaustionPositive and depression (−)YesNegative and depression (+)Positive and complaints (−)Negative and anxiety (+)Negative, more potentSocial support, social conflict33 (Men with HIV; convenient; 50% black)Additive effectsSpearman correlationsDepression, mood, lonelinessPositive and mental health (+)YesNegative and mental health (−)Supportive relationships, problematic relationships458 (married; young adults; community; 90% white)Additive effectsMultiple regressionDepressionPositive and depression (−)YesNegative and depression (+)Negative, more potentPositive social inter-actions, negative social interactions178 (middle-aged and older adults; national probability; 89% white)Domain-specificStructural equation modelingPsychological well-beingPositive and positive well-being (+)NoNegative and negative well-being (+)Equal effectsNetwork helpfulness, network upset68 (caregivers; convenient; white)Additive effectsMANOVA, hierarchical multiple regressionDepressionPositive and depression (N.S.)NoNegative and depression (+)Negative, more potentSocial support, negative social interaction101 (college students; convenient)Additive effectsCorrelationsPsychological distress, positive affectivityNegative and distress (+)…Equal effectsSocial support, social conflict228 (college students; room-mates; convenient; 4% black)Additive effects, cross-domain bufferingCorrelations, regressionPsychological distressPositive and distress (+)YesNegative and distress (−)Positive × negative (significant)Equal effectsPositive social inter-actions, negative social interactions1,201 (younger and older adults; national probability)Additive effects, bufferingMultiple regressionDepressionPositive and depression (−)YesNegative and depression (+)Positive × negative (significant)Positive, more potentNetwork helpfulness, network upset68 (37–85 years old; caregivers; convenient)Additive effects, moderatorHierarchical multiple regressionDepressionPositive and depression (N.S.)…Negative and depression (+)Positive × negative (significant)Negative, more potentPositive social inter-actions, negative social interactions106 (caregivers; convenient; 98% white)Additive effects, moderatorMixed model/chunkwise regression, moderated multiple regressionDistress, depressionPositive and distress (N.S.)…Negative and distress (−)Negative and depression (−)Positive × stress (N.S.)Negative × stress (significant)Negative, more potentPositive social support, negative social support207 (patients with chronic fatigue syndrome)Additive effectsCorrelationsDepression, anxietyPositive and anxiety (−)YesNegative and anxiety (+)Negative and depression (+)Negative, more potentPositive support, problematic support101 (arthritis patients; convenient; 14% black)Additive effects, stress- bufferingCorrelations, hierarchial multiple regressionDepressionPositive and depression (−)NoNegative and depression (+)Positive × negative (significant)Positive × stress (N.S.)Negative × stress (N.S.)Equal effectsSupportive interactions, problematic interactions129 (single mothers; convenient; 100% black)Additive effects, moderatorHierarchial multiple regressionPsychological distressPositive and distress (N.S.)YesNegative and distress (−)Negative × stress (significant)Negative, more potentSupportive social ties, problematic social ties115 (elderly widows; convenient; 97% white)Additive effectsMultiple regressionPsychological well-beingNegative and psychological well-being (−)NoPositive and psychological well-being (N.S.)Negative, more potentSocial support, social hindrance229 (undergrads; convenient)Additive effectsHierarchical multiple regressionPsychological distress, psychological well-beingPositive and distress (N.S.)NoNegative and distress (+)Negative, more potentSupportive interaction, negative interaction1,755 (married couples; nonblack; community)Additive effectsMultiple regressionDepressionPositive and distress (−)YesNegative and distress (+)Negative, more potentPositive network inter-actions, negative network interactions83 (gay men; convenient; 11% black)Additive effects; stress- bufferingHierarchical regressionDepressionPositive and depression (−)…Negative and depression (+)Positive × stress (N.S.)Negative × stress (significant)Equal effectsWanted/needed inter-actions, unwanted/unneeded inter-actions48 (simple random; convenient; 12.5% black)Additive effectsMultiple regressionPsychiatric symptoms; moralePositive and morale (N.S.)NoNegative and morale (−)Positive and symptoms (N.S.)Negative and symptoms (+)Negative, more potentSocial support, social undermining1,630 (unemployed adults and spouse/partner; community; 21.5% black)Additive effectsStructural equation modelingDepressionPositive and depression (−)YesNegative and depression (+)Effects equalSocial support, social undermining1,087 (unemployed adults; community; 15% black)Additive effectsStructural equation modelingDepression, anxietyNegative and anxiety (+)YesNegative and depression (+)Negative, more potent

Open in a separate window

NOTE.—Information in the “Sample” column describes characteristics of the sample. Various study designs were used: “convenient” describes samples taken from a convenience sample questionnaire; “community,” samples taken from community-based studies; and “national probablity,” samples taken from national probability surveys. The “Results” column provides information regarding the relationship between social interactions and well-being. The symbols in parentheses indicate whether the relationship between a given social interaction and well-being is positive (+) or negative (−). N.S. = not significant.

Few works have attempted to discuss whether positive and negative interactions form a single dimension, but some have assumed as much. The purpose of estimating the relationship between these two concepts is to determine whether positive and negative aspects of social interaction are distinct from one another. An individual may have many interactions of one kind and few of the other if positive and negative interactions are related. However, if the two concepts are not related, then an individual may have frequent interactions of both kinds, few interactions of both kinds, or any combination thereof. Further, if there is no relationship, then the next step is to determine which aspect of an interaction is more significant for psychological well-being.

Of those studies that measure both concepts separately, very few show strong relations between positive and negative interactions, which thus indirectly supports the view that the two concepts are distinct dimensions. Further, although correlation between variables is not proof of one dimension, investigations using structural equation modeling (e.g., ; ; ) suggest that positive and negative social interactions constitute empirically distinct constructs rather than parts of the same factor. Of the 28 studies reviewed, 20 (71 percent) estimated the correlation between positive and negative social interactions. Eleven of these 20 studies (55 percent) found a significant correlation between the two concepts, while the remaining nine studies (45 percent) failed to find a significant correlation. Although there may be several reasons for this discrepancy in the findings, including theoretical and methodological differences, it is the conceptual differences that are particularly interesting, specifically, the source of interaction.

A clear pattern emerges among studies that investigate the relationship between positive and negative social interactions. Of the 11 studies that found a significant correlation, all but one used a source-specific approach as opposed to a global approach to measure social interaction. That is, those investigations using a source-specific approach examined the impact of positive and negative social interactions on psychological well-being from several different sources in an individual’s network, including spouses, relatives, and friends (; ; ); spouses only (; ; ); people at varying degrees of closeness (); friends and roommates (); and coworkers and supervisors (). This approach directly addresses the question of whether interaction with different sets of individuals has differential effects on psychological well-being and emphasizes the importance of distinguishing between source-specific and global studies in addressing the controversy. In any case, results from these studies do indeed suggest that each concept has distinct effects on well-being when presented by source, and thus both positive and negative interactions deserve to be investigated separately, given that they are unlikely to be one dimension.

The nine studies reporting no correlation between positive and negative social interactions did not take a source-specific approach. These studies attempted to capture the multifaceted nature of the support concept by assessing social interactions across various dimensions of support irrespective of source (i.e., global approach). In fact, although two of these studies (; ) examined the unique effects of particular dimensions of social interaction, the remaining studies aggregated the various dimensions and, in doing so, lost the ability to determine the unique effect of particular dimensions of social interaction on psychological well-being (; ; ; ; ), making it impossible to determine the effects of separate dimensions of supportive interaction.

The relationship between positive and negative social interactions clearly warrants further investigation. However, a review of the relevant research suggests that the current debate about the relationship between positive and negative social interactions may reflect the conceptualization of these constructs. Apparently, studies that focus on the relationship with a particular individual (e.g., spouse, relative, friend) will have very different results from those that focus on relationships with individuals in general, or more global networks.

The varying results from these studies demonstrate the importance of considering different sources, as well as different dimensions of supportive and negative relations. Perhaps this debate can be reconciled by research that simultaneously assesses various dimensions of supportive relationships and different sources of support. It is reasonable to speculate that different sources of social interaction and different dimensions of social support can have differential effects on psychological well-being. There is some evidence that both the source and dimension of support should be considered when attempting to determine the effect of supportive interactions on health and well-being (e.g., ). For example, review of the current social support and illness research, not covered here, reveals two specific types of supportive interactions perceived as being stressful: (1) when the patient (or individual) perceives the wrong person as having provided a particular type of support and (2) when the patient (or individual) feels that although the correct person has provided the support, they have not done so in a manner perceived as helpful (; ; ). In either of these two types of interaction, the effect can be negative. Therefore, researchers need to specify that both dimensions and sources of supportive interactions may play a key role in determining the effects of positive and negative social interactions on psychological well-being.

Conceptual Models of Relations

Although the literature concerning the simultaneous effects of positive and negative social interactions on psychological well-being is small and equivocal, three general conceptual models of relations between support and psychological well-being have been identified that are useful for organizing these studies. The following section describes these models: (1) the additive or direct effects model, (2) the moderator or buffering model, and (3) the domain-specific model.

The Additive Effects Model

The additive or direct effects model suggests that positive and negative social interactions have additive effects on psychological well-being. This perspective argues that positive interactions have a beneficial impact on well-being, while negative interactions have a harmful impact. Although all 28 of the studies reviewed used an additive effects approach to their investigation, a variety of methodological and analytic strategies were used to test this theoretical model. For example, the samples used were quite diverse, representing differences in race, age, marital and socioeconomic status, sexual orientation, and health status. Further, various study designs were used, including national probability surveys, community-based questionnaire designs, and convenience samples. Finally, several different statistical procedures were used to test the additive effects model, including correlations, various multiple regression procedures, and structural equation modeling. These methodological and analytic differences partly explain the inconsistent findings across the various studies.

The prevailing view among researchers is that negative social interactions have a more potent effect on psychological well-being than do positive interactions (e.g., ). It has even been argued that the absence of negative social interactions may be more important for health and well-being than the presence of positive social interactions (). Research in this area has typically found that negative interactions occur less frequently than positive interactions but are often more predictive of mental health outcomes. However, some evidence suggests that positive social interactions are more important for positive mental health outcomes (), while other work indicates that positive and negative social interactions have equal effects on mental health outcomes (; ).

Nineteen of the studies listed in table A1 (68 percent) reported that negative social interactions had a stronger impact on psychological well-being, while only one study reported stronger effects for positive social interactions. Six of the studies (21 percent) reported equal effects of positive and negative social interactions on psychological well-being. This disparity in the research makes the task of integrating and comparing findings across studies difficult. No observable trends in conceptualization, operationalization, samples, or analytic strategy were found in those studies indicating that negative social interactions were more potent, nor were they found among those reporting comparable effects for positive and negative interactions. However, these results provide strong support for the argument that negative social interactions have more potent effects on psychological well-being than positive interactions. Still, the number of studies reporting contradictory findings cannot be ignored.

To make sense of the work that has been done so far in this area, we need a fully articulated theory of how positive and negative social interactions either help or hinder psychological well-being. A better understanding of the relationship between social interactions and well-being will be useful for developing tools for social support assessment and designing effective interventions that include members of a clients’ social network. Most of the studies examined here did not have an identified theoretical framework. In fact, less than 20 percent of the studies explicitly identified the theoretical model to be tested. Given the various fields and disciplines (e.g., sociology, psychology, anthropology, epidemiology), different methodological approaches (e.g., large population-based surveys, community-based studies, convenience-sample questionnaire designs), and differences in populations studied (e.g., college students and middle-aged and older adults), it is not surprising that the progress has been less than orderly and that our understanding of the relationships among positive and negative social interactions and psychological well-being is limited.

Operational complexities may explain the inconsistent findings. Several different measures of positive and negative social interactions were employed across investigations. Some researchers used standardized instruments to measure positive social interactions (e.g., ), while others consulted previous research when selecting their measures for both positive and negative social interactions. Further, only nine studies (32 percent) employed comparable items for positive and negative social interactions, both in number and content. A lack of comparability between measures of positive and negative aspects of social relations may lead to disparate findings regarding the relationships among social interactions and psychological well-being, in particular whether positive and negative relations are distinct dimensions and the relative potency of the effects of the two concepts on well-being.

The conceptualization of psychological well-being may also partly explain the equivocal findings in the literature. For example, although many of the investigators identified depression as one of their outcome measures (64 percent), others identified psychological distress (21 percent), psychological well-being (11 percent), and various other dimensions of well-being, including anxiety (11 percent), psychiatric symptoms (7 percent), positive and negative affect (3 percent), morale (3 percent), emotional well-being (3 percent), and life satisfaction (3 percent). Further, several investigators identified a common dimension of psychological well-being (e.g., depression) but measured the concept in different ways (e.g., CES-D, Hopkins Symptoms Checklist, Beck Depression Inventory [BDI], the Symptom Checklist-90-R [SCL-90-R]).

The Moderator Model

Similar to the stress-buffering model described by , the moderator model assumes that positive social interactions buffer the deleterious impact of negative social interactions on psychological well-being. Studies using this model tended to examine one of three things: (1) the extent to which positive social interactions buffered the impact of stress on psychological well-being (e.g., ); (2) whether negative social interactions could be potentially buffered by positive social interactions (; ); and (3) the extent to which negative social interactions exacerbated the effects of stress on psychological well-being (). As illustrated in table A1, 10 studies were identified that examined the interaction effect of positive and negative social interactions. The following section discusses how positive and negative relations interact to moderate the relationship between social interactions and psychological well-being. Consistent with the argument that the source can potentially influence the outcome, the following section is divided into same-source (e.g., positive and negative interactions with one’s spouse), cross-domain (e.g., positive interactions with one’s friend and negative interactions with one’s relative), and non-source-specific (e.g., general or global measures of positive and negative interactions) investigations.

Same-source buffering

Seven of the studies reviewed (25 percent) used source-specific measures of positive and negative social interactions. However, only four of the seven studies (57 percent) reported buffering effects when positive and negative social interactions involved the same source. In a study by , social support buffered conflict for “some one person.” That is, for respondents who reported receiving low levels of social support from one person, the relationship between social conflict from that individual and emotional well-being was strong and significant. In contrast, for those reporting high levels of social support from “some one person,” the relationship between conflict and well-being was nonexistent. examined the degree to which positive interactions with parents buffered the effect of negative interactions with parents on daughters’ level of depression. No significant interaction was observed. However, the results were different when positive and negative interactions were disaggregated and maternal and paternal interactions were examined along with parental interactions. Specifically, positive and negative interactions with one’s father interacted with one another in their association with depression. This finding suggests if an individual has high levels of support from his or her father, the impact of negative interactions with one’s father on depression may be attenuated.

also found that when positive social interactions with an individual’s spouse are low, negative social interactions with the spouse adversely affect depressive symptoms. The same results were observed when relatives and friends were the source examined. Finally, investigated the degree to which positive and negative social interactions decreased or increased the effects of economic strain (e.g., difficulty paying bills, worrying about money, not having enough money for medical care) on psychological distress among young African-American mothers. A significant interaction effect was found between non-kin relationship problems and economic strain. Specifically, as economic strain and problems with friends increased, levels of psychological distress also increased.

found evidence of a “reverse” buffering effect among older adults for positive and negative social interactions with other relatives or friends. Specifically, high levels of negative interactions were not influenced by positive interactions. However, when levels of negative interactions were low, increasing levels of positive interactions reduced the impact of negative interactions on depression. This finding is quite interesting and suggests that high conflict with a network member “neutralizes” the potential benefits of positive social interactions with that same member (). At low levels of conflict, however, positive exchanges are a more effective buffer.

Cross-source buffering

Two studies found evidence to support a cross-source or different source hypothesis that credits one source’s interactions (e.g., family relations) with buffering negative interactions with another source (e.g., friend). found only cross-domain buffering effects of social support, such that support from friends cushioned the impact of conflict with roommates on psychological distress. Similarly, support from roommates cushioned the impact of conflict with friends on distress. found that among older adults (60–92 years), positive social interactions with children buffered the harmful effects on depressive symptoms of negative social interactions with both spouse and other relatives or friends. Likewise, the effect of negative social interactions with children on depressive symptoms was buffered by positive social interactions with other relatives or friends.

Non-source-specific buffering

Measures that did not specify a particular source of interaction (e.g., spouse, relative, friend) but employed a more general measure were used in the four remaining studies, which reported significant interaction effects of positive and negative social interactions on psychological well-being. and found that, although positive interactions did not buffer the effect of stress on distress and depression, negative interactions amplified the effect of stress on distress and depression. In contrast, the final two studies found evidence to support the buffering hypothesis. found that negative interactions were associated with increased depression for patients who received little positive support. Similarly, found negative interactions to be associated with increasing depression among caregivers who reported positive interactions with their network. This suggests that positive interactions with networks may be expected, but that negative interactions are unexpected. Thus, negative interactions may be more salient events than positive interactions and may have harmful effects on psychological well-being.

The Domain-Specific Model

The domain-specific model assumes that positive and negative social interactions have equal effects on psychological well-being within their respective domain. That is, positive aspects of the social network are hypothesized to be associated with positive well-being, and negative aspects are hypothesized to be associated with negative well-being. tested four conceptual models that potentially characterize the relationships among positive and negative social interactions and psychological well-being. Using non-source-specific measures of social interaction (i.e., global measures), they found that, among middle-aged and older adults, positive and negative social interactions have equal effects on psychological well-being within their respective social domain. Specifically, positive aspects of the network (e.g., confiding, reassurance, getting respect) were associated with positive aspects of well-being (e.g., feeling excited, proud, pleased), and negative aspects of the network (e.g., getting on nerves, too demanding) were associated with negative aspects of well-being (e.g., feeling restless, depressed, upset).

The results of illustrate the complex relationships among positive and negative social interactions and psychological well-being. The domain-specific model accounts for two dimensions of social interaction and two dimensions of psychological well-being. This theoretical model challenges findings that suggest more potent effects of one aspect of social interaction over the other and calls into question using an outcome measure that represents only one dimension of psychological well-being.

A variety of alternative models have been proposed to explain the association between positive and negative social interactions and their effect on psychological well-being. The conceptual models presented here—the additive effects, moderator, and domain-specific models—represent only three ways that social interactions can potentially affect psychological well-being. Currently, little empirical research exists that examines the utility of these models. Moreover, the lack of conceptual and methodological consensus encourages the proliferation of alternative models, without settling current debates about the nature of the relationships among positive and negative social interaction and psychological well-being.

Implications for Social Work Practice and Research

Social workers have long recognized the importance of an individual’s social network for health and well-being. In direct practice, one must accurately assess the client’s supportive network before planning any kind of intervention. Assessment is defined as the process of “gathering, analyzing, and synthesizing salient data into a formulation that encompasses the following vital dimensions: 1) the nature of clients’ problems, including special attention to the roles that clients and significant others play in the difficulties; 2) the functioning (strengths, limitations, personality assets, and deficiencies) of clients and significant others; 3) the motivation of clients to work on the problems; 4) the relevant environmental factors that contribute to the problems; and 5) the resources that are available or are needed to ameliorate clients’ difficulties” (, p. 193). This definition of assessment involves both the individual and his or her social network. It follows, then, that social workers need to deliberately and comprehensively assess the significant forces in their clients’ social networks before providing services or designing interventions.

A comprehensive assessment recognizes that primary relationships within a client’s social network can be sources of both help and stress at different times and under different conditions. Therefore, social workers must assess both the quantity and the quality of social interactions. Quantity refers to the actual number of social interactions, and the quality of one’s network refers to how rewarding and satisfying a client’s social relationships are. Social workers tend to romanticize or idealize the role of supportive networks. A more realistic perspective is needed, as well as instruments that comprehensively assess the social networks of individuals. Simply assessing the structure of a clients’ social network (e.g., size or number) provides little information about the quality, amount, and experience of positive and negative social interactions (). For example, small networks may at first seem inadequate, but the client may perceive them as very supportive and have very few negative interactions with them. In contrast, large networks, although seemingly ideal for its ability to provide support, may be perceived by the client as unsupportive. Social workers need to assess the quality of interactions, in addition to the number of available supportive resources.

A great deal of progress has been made in assessing social support (see ). Despite this progress, social support assessment remains an underdeveloped area. Because it is difficult to formulate a definition of social support that includes all or most of its dimensions, it is even more challenging to develop instruments based on such a definition. The lack of universal terms to describe supportive social relations, along with the lack of understanding of the mechanisms by which these relations influence health and well-being, has made it burdensome to integrate the disparate research findings obtained from the various disciplines. The complexity of social support processes requires several different measures. Many instruments do not cover more than one dimension, or they confuse or aggregate the dimensions, and most instruments fail to take into account the negative aspects of supportive relations. Further, the negative aspects of the social support process, though recognized, have not been well-integrated into current measures of social support assessment ().

An additional challenge to accurately assessing social support and social networks stems from many social support assessment tools being too complex, lengthy, difficult to administer, and not designed for direct practice (). Generally lacking an integrating theoretical foundation, social network assessment tools currently used by social work practitioners are numerous and are based on a variety of approaches that may not be appropriate for specific practice situations. Not only is more research needed to understand the processes whereby social interactions affect psychological well-being, but research is also needed to develop more appropriate instruments to accurately assess social support in direct practice with individuals and families and that have a relatively high level of clinical utility (i.e., short, inexpensive, and easy to administer).

Directions for Future Research

Before research on the relationship between social support, social networks, and psychological well-being can advance, a clear definition of terms is needed in order to gain a better understanding of positive and negative interactions and psychological well-being. A review of the literature reveals a plethora of terms and conceptualizations for these constructs. Positive and negative interactions and psychological well-being may refer to many constructs, defined in different ways, that play important and different roles in our lives. More precise definitions will facilitate research investigating these potential roles and in integrating the disparate findings.

Future research efforts should also focus on the various types of positive and negative social interactions and how these may vary across age-groups or cultural groups. Qualitative techniques, such as ethnographic studies and focus groups, should be conducted with diverse populations in order to determine the kinds of positive and negative interactions that individuals identify as helpful or harmful. Age, socioeconomic status, and race are just a few of the factors that may influence how positive and negative interactions are defined, as well as their impact on psychological well-being.

Future work must also develop a theoretical framework for understanding the processes whereby social interactions affect psychological well-being. Several theoretical models were presented that potentially characterize the relationships among these constructs. These models may, in fact, depict the diverse ways in which positive and negative interactions are related to well-being. But, very few empirical investigations have been conducted to test these alternative models. Therefore, the mechanisms through which positive and negative social interactions affect psychological well-being remain unclear. Further, it is unclear whether negative interactions actually have more potent effects on well-being than positive interactions. This review of the literature identified 19 articles reporting stronger effects for negative interactions, compared with only one article reporting stronger effects for positive interactions. One possible explanation is that negative interactions are more rare and thus more salient than positive ones, which may be anticipated. Or, perhaps negative interactions erode feelings of personal control or self-esteem, decrease motivation to engage in positive health behaviors, or provoke adverse physiological responses (; ).

There is the possibility that negative interactions are conceptualized in such a way that results in their greater impact on psychological well-being. Some researchers have argued that investigations of the joint effects of positive and negative social interactions do not employ equivalent measures of the concepts (; ). For example, measures of positive interactions may comprise quantitative aspects (e.g., number of network members, frequency of contact), while measures of negative interactions include qualitative dimensions (e.g., degree of negativity within a network). Researchers should include parallel measures of positive and negative social interactions in investigations. Additional studies that employ parallel measures of psychological well-being are also needed. Research examining positive and negative dimensions of both social interaction and psychological well-being will greatly contribute to the literature.

Finally, researchers need to develop strong assessment tools. A review of available social support assessment instruments () revealed the scarcity of tools that include measures of negative social interactions. Limitations related to theoretical and conceptual issues in social support research have inhibited the development of tools that comprehensively assess social support networks. Further, the lack of attention to negative aspects of supportive relations in the literature has resulted in the omission of such measures from available assessment instruments. To strengthen assessment, researchers must develop social support assessment instruments that are theoretically and conceptually linked to the dimensions of social interaction being measured; improve the reliability and validity of existing instruments; standardize existing measures so that assessment results will be comparable across diverse groups (; ); and develop assessment tools that incorporate both positive and negative social interactions.

Acknowledgments

I wish to thank Robert Joseph Taylor for his helpful comments and suggestions, as well as Linda M. Chatters for her comments on earlier drafts.

Footnotes

1A total of three articles reviewed for this article included a sample of adolescents; two of them examined the interactions between adolescents and adults and one included both adolescents and adults in the investigation.

What is interaction in a concept of society?

A social interaction is an exchange between two or more individuals and is a building block of society. Social interaction can be studied between groups of two (dyads), three (triads) or larger social groups. By interacting with one another, people design rules, institutions and systems within which they seek to live.

What concept means level of interaction?

Level of Interaction: is a metric used in eLearning that refers to concepts such as user interaction, complexity and level of sophistication of the course.

Who gave the concept of social interaction?

Erving Goffman's (1959) concept of impression management, discussed in the text, is one of the key sociological insights for the understanding of social interaction.

Which refers to two or more persons who are socially interacting with one another?

In the social sciences, a social group can be defined as two or more people who interact with one another, share similar characteristics, and collectively have a sense of unity.